the infant becomes an adult person. Obviously, relative to the wild dog, the
human requires some type of intensive support infrastructure for a long period
of time, many years in fact. Small wonder, then, that the social environments
of humans, from which this long-term support derives, are quite distinct from
the lower animals.
The infant
bonobo (Pan paniscus) has quite similar development characteristics to
the human, with the exception that from birth it possesses the physical
dexterity to grasp its mother’s fur in order to be carried about the arboreal
environment in which these close relatives of the human species live. They are
considered close relatives in that they shared a common ancestor species about
6 million years ago. Beyond this earlier (relative to humans) ability to attach
itself to its mother and hence travel about, the infant bonobo develops in a
manner and time frame that bears significant similarities to that of an infant
human. Frans de Waal has shown in Chimpanzee Politics and Bonobo
(with Frans Lanting) that the base family groupings of bonobo and chimpanzees
bear similarity to that for humans. It is an obvious divergence of their
respective evolutionary paths that humans went beyond the basic extended family
in developing the mechanisms to support more complex groupings while these apes
did not.
As the human
species emerged, the basic family group provided the support infrastructure in
which the young could survive and develop. The most basic requirements for this
survival were safety and protection from the elements and from predators along
with the provision of food and water. In a hunter gatherer environment, a
reasonable assessment of the value of the group in any form is the ability to
focus effort and resources to the most pressing problem at any point in time,
and the ability to share the results of the attempt to solve these problems
across the entire group. A mother or some other caretaker could remain with an
infant while others in the group searched for food. On finding food, it could
then be shared with the rest of the group. If the group could adapt to the
physical ecosystem, then it could optimize its prospects for finding food, or
shelter or safety, perhaps by subdividing the group. If one hunting party found
game, then the entire group might eat. If one hunting party failed, then
perhaps the entire group did not have to starve. If a hunting party was out on
the hunt, then others might remain with the young as protection. The grouping
mechanism of the extended family unit was beneficial as long as the physical
ecosystem could support it.
In a
hunter-gatherer system, the physical ecosystem almost certainly places a limit
on the size of group that can be supported in the basic mode; that is, in the
mode of searching for sustenance and protection. Individual members of the
group, or other small units, might tend to split off to form other groups or to
integrate themselves into other existing groups. In any case, many groups would
likely find themselves derived from a single, or small number of base family
units and hence might offer some semblance of multi-group cohesion; essentially
providing ground for a larger scale grouping mechanism in the form of a clan.
During the human
species emergent period, as the extended family unit exceeded the nominal size
that can be supported by its geographic extent within the physical ecosystem,
the group may have tended to become less advantageous, and rather began to pay
something of a price for its excessive size, and may have divided into multiple
units in order for each smaller, derivative group to retain the competitive
advantage offered by the family grouping mechanism. Over evolutionary time, it
may well have transpired that multi-level selection benefits could accrue to
even larger, multiple related family units if some new means of coordination of
group activities
|