We assume that
since the human species’ earliest days of emergence, when all its members lived
in the wild with all other species, the physiological needs and capabilities of
the human infant have not changed significantly. That is, since the beginning
of the species, the human infant has been not only totally helpless, it has
always constituted a burden upon its mother or any other humans that felt
compelled to sustain its life. An infant today cannot protect itself from the
simplest of dangers and our assumption is that it has always been thus. For
example, any number of other species, ranging from ants or other insects on the
ground or in the bushes, to the major predators of the day (lions and tigers
and bears; oh my!) are all threats to the human infant. The infant has
virtually no defensive capabilities beyond the fact that, in most cases, its
mother and father feel a natural compulsion to provide for their infant. While
this results in a realization of the infant’s need to be carried and protected
by larger humans, it also results in the inhibition of these humans’ ability to
hunt for or gather food. The species has evolved mechanisms of grouping
together for the enhanced protection and support of these individuals; very
specifically the infants. However, the manner in which the design for these
mechanisms is conveyed from generation to generation is still unclear. It does
seem more than plausible that the conveyance mechanism does exist. Through the
evolutionary process, the basic human group (the family) developed around the
enhanced ability to protect the individual members of the group, beyond their
individual capabilities to protect themselves, and to garner sufficient food
for all the members of the group beyond what they were able to garner
individually.
It seems most plausible that this earliest grouping formed around what
today we know as the basic nuclear family; a man and a woman with a
child or children. Of course, there is also documented evidence, for example in
Australia’s outback, of
larger family units in prehistoric settings. There, as the children grow, mate
and have additional children, the family itself grows, perhaps with the result
that it becomes multi-generational and perhaps even multi-familial. Similar
social structures are still found preferentially in rural America, although
their prevalence has diminished since the middle of the XXth Century.
While the basic family unit is arguably grounded in physiological principles
derived from established evolutionary processes, large groupings involve
enhanced communication and social skills on the part of members of the human
species. This leads to the establishment of larger and more complex groups. We
identify these larger groups as first, clans, then tribes and
then very large-scale groups. We refer to the largest human groups as congregations
and then as égalité. While we don’t have a firm history of this
development, the existence of all such groups throughout recorded history is a
strong indication that each revealed a successful evolutionary mechanism. That
is, the emergence of groups certainly enhanced the survival characteristics of
the individual; they enhanced the individual’s natural selection chances.
Moreover, according to some researchers, such as David Sloan Wilson in Darwin’s
Cathedral, a variety of indicators suggest that among-group selection is at
work as well; that groups compete and that certain group characteristics
enhance the natural selection chances of specific groups beyond that simply of
the individuals within the groups.
In this case, the principles of natural selection are applied in a
recursive fashion, thus giving rise to group-selection as well as
individual-selection processes. With multi-level selection processes as the
backdrop, our consideration of ecosystems must keep apace. Thus, we would like
to consider the structure of ecosystems in which complex selection mechanisms
for humans can function and then consider comparable ecosystems for computers
and computer infrastructure, including an amalgam of the two. We characterize
this extension of the basic concepts of an ecosystem as a social ecosystem. From time to time, throughout the remainder of
this book, we may refer to the original concept of an ecosystem as a physical
ecosystem in order to unambiguously differentiate it from a social
ecosystem. If we use the word ecosystem alone, we are generally referring to a
physical ecosystem.
|