with capabilities that
obviously separate us in degree of function or form of function from our
mammalian predecessors, would it be art, mathematics, religion or science. It
is natural to look for capabilities underlying those forms of expression in the
mechanisms of the neocortex. Various types of symbolic manipulation associated
with their expression in speech and other human-specific communications are
part of the neocortex contribution to human cognition. We will later consider
more fully the question of how this contribution relates to that of the other
parts of the triune brain.
Causation is the
establishment of symbolic intermediaries in the observation of correlation.
Observable early in infancy, causality is sought for, which is equivalent to
saying that a satisfying chemical balance in the brain accompanies the establishment
of cause. One of the evolutionary advantages of causality is planning. Causality can be recognized
directly (“He fell because someone pushed him.”) or indirectly (“He fell, and I
assume that someone pushed him.”). Causes can also be unobservable (“He fell,
and I didn’t observe anyone, so a spirit may have pushed him.”). As for other
functions, the establishment of unobservable causes is of survival benefit if
it leads to better fitness (“I avoid the spirit, and I don’t fall.”). The
build-up of a causal chain can lead humans to gods, and then to God. If the
causal chain is invalid, it can take us over the precipice.
Just as
elementary causes (spirits) may offer a better theory for survival, more
elaborate, unobservable causes can lead to yet other theories that are more
complex. Some would consider, in resonance with the thesis of René Girard
developed in several books starting with Violence
and the Sacred, that monotheistic theories, judged by their historical
expansion, have expressed a particular fitness in allowing societies to build
and expand in our most recent history. The build-up of causation is based on
imitation, itself apparently implemented with mirror neurons, as described by
Giacomo Rizzolati, Leonardo Fogassi and Vittorio Gallese Mirrors in the Mind Scientific American article. Computers use
state machines for communication in the same way. State machines are shared by
two communicating computers. A state machine makes predictions based on
historical performance, which is saying that it represents a causal model or
develops a theory on the behavior of the state machine with which it is
communicating. This forms a component of the causal chain, complemented by
metaphoric capabilities that we review in some detail later in this chapter.
The evolutionary
progression of computers over the first half-century or so of their electronic
incarnation has been replete with competitive interplay among a variety of
software architectures. We have made the point that identifying the analogue of
computer systems relative to the human mind is not quite as simple as
differentiating computer hardware from software. When the model of social
ecosystems is mapped onto computer systems and their networks, the trust and
policy infrastructures cross the boundaries between hardware and software. A somewhat
similar effect is noted within the brain, where higher cognitive functions are
spread across broad regions. That said, an interesting decomposition to
recognize and attempt to understand, at least in overview, is that of the
dichotomy within computer software between operating systems and applications.
Historically,
computers have tended to be general-purpose tools. A specific computer platform
might be expected to support work on a number of problems, perhaps sequentially
or perhaps in parallel. It might be expected to work on these problems for a
single person, or it might be expected to work on behalf of many different
people. Moreover, it might be expected to work on
|